网络资源的拷贝粘贴 备份参考之用


8 February 2009

Difference Between Asperger's Syndrome and High Functioning Autism - Dr Tony Attwood's View

Is There a Difference Between Asperger's Syndrome and High Functioning Autism?

Dr Tony Attwood

We have been exploring the nature of autism, as described by Leo Kanner, for nearly 60
years. He described a severe form of autism, typified by the silent and aloof child. We
have only been exploring the profile of autism described by Hans Asperger for about 15
years. The children he described had speech and were active participants in social
interactions. There is currently some debate in the academic literature and between
clinicians as to whether Asperger's syndrome is a unique disorder with a profile of
abilities that does not occur in any other syndrome or simply a form of autism with a
higher intelligence quotient.

There is general agreement that autism as defined by Leo Kanner and 'autistic
psychopathy' (the original descriptive term of Hans Asperger which was later changed
to the term Asperger's syndrome by Lorna Wing) are two conditions within the range of
disorders known as Pervasive Developmental Disorders or Autistic Spectrum Disorders.
In 1994 the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV) provided
diagnostic criteria for Asperger's syndrome. The opinion of the authors of the manual,
which was revised in 2000 (DSM-IV-TR) was that Asperger's syndrome could be
differentiated from autism by an examination of the child's early development and the
existence of some characteristics that were rare in children with autism. They considered
that early language and cognitive skills are not delayed significantly in children with
Asperger's syndrome. There is also no clinically significant delay in age-appropriate selfhelp
skills, adaptive behaviour and curiosity about the environment in childhood. The
clinical profile of a child with Asperger's syndrome is also less likely to include motor
mannerisms and preoccupation with parts of objects as occurs in autism but the child
can have a circumscribed interest that consumes a great deal of their time amassing
information and facts. They also noted that the profile of social skills in children with
autism includes self-isolation or rigid social approaches, while in Asperger's syndrome
there can be a motivation to socialise but this is achieved in a highly eccentric, one-sided,
verbose and insensitive manner. Should the child's profile of abilities and developmental
history be consistent with the criteria for both autism and Asperger's syndrome, the
authors of the DSM state that a diagnosis of autism should take precedence.

The diagnostic criteria in the DSM, which provide a differentiation between autism and
Asperger's syndrome, have been examined by several research studies over the last five
years. There has been some criticism from clinicians and research that the criteria do not
identify the disorder Hans Asperger originally described. The four cases he described in
his original paper would be diagnosed, according to DSM criteria, as having autism not
Asperger's syndrome. (Miller and Ozonoff 1997). If one was to use the DSM criteria,
Asperger's syndrome would be a very rare condition.

Research has also been conducted on whether delayed language in children with autism
can accurately predict later clinical symptoms. Three studies have cast considerable
doubt over the use of early language delay as a differential criterion between autism and
Asperger's syndrome (Eisenmajer, Prior, Leekam, Wing, Ong, Gould and Welham 1998,
Dickerson Mayes and Calhoun 2001 Manjiviona and Prior 1999). Any differences in
language ability that are apparent in the pre-school years between children with autism
and Asperger's syndrome has largely disappeared by early adolescence (Eisenmajer,
Prior, Leekam, Wing, Ong, Gould and Welham 1998, Ozonoff, South and Miller 2000).
There is general agreement that children with Asperger's syndrome may not show any
conspicuous cognitive delay in early childhood. Indeed, some can be quite precocious or
talented in terms of learning to read, numerical abilities and in some aspects of their
constructive play and memory. Children with autism can be recognised as having
developmental delay in their cognitive abilities from infancy and diagnosed as young as
18 months of age with a mean age of diagnosis of five years. Children with Asperger's
syndrome are often not diagnosed until after they start school with a mean age of
diagnosis of eleven years (Howlin and Asgharian 1999). However, the signs of
Asperger's syndrome in very young children may be more subtle and easily
camouflaged at home and school. On reflection, parents (especially mothers) and
teachers have often been concerned about some aspects of the child's cognitive
development, in particular their social reasoning, but their concerns may have been
intuitive, and difficult to describe to clinicians. It is not until the child is expected to
show more advanced cognitive abilities that formal assessments indicate significant
delay or an unusual profile in cognitive development.

There has been research comparing the cognitive profile of adolescents with autism and
Asperger's syndrome. The studies have examined the cognitive profile of what may be
called 'High Functioning Autism', that is children with a diagnosis of autism with an
Intelligence Quotient in the normal range, i.e. above 70. The term High Functioning
Autism has been used in the past to describe children who had the classic signs of
autism in early childhood but as they developed, formal testing of their cognitive skills
indicated a greater degree of intellectual ability with greater social and adaptive
behaviour skills than are usual with children with autism. Their clinical outcome was
better than expected. The cognitive abilities of this group of children were then
compared to the cognitive profile of children with Asperger's syndrome, who did not
have a history of early cognitive or language delay. The results of the research has not
established a distinct and consistent profile for each group. Ehlers, Nyden, Gillberg,
Dahlgren Sanberg, Dahlgren, Hjelmquist and Oden (1997) found that only a minority of
each diagnostic group showed a characteristic profile.

One group of researchers, based at Yale University in the United States have suggested,
on the basis of their research studies, that the neuropsychological profiles of children
with Asperger's syndrome and High Functioning Autism are different. (Klin, Volkmar,
Sparrow, Cicchetti and Rourke 1995). However, research by other scientists examining
diagnostic differentiation using neuropsychological testing has not identified a distinct
profile that discriminates between the two groups. (Manjiviona and Prior 1999, Miller
and Ozonoff 2000 Ozonoff South and Miller 2000).

The DSM criteria refer to children with Asperger's syndrome as having, in comparison
to children with autism, no clinically significant delay in age-appropriate self-help skills
and adaptive behaviour. Clinical experience indicates that parents, especially mothers of
children and adolescents with Asperger's syndrome, often have to provide verbal
reminders and advice regarding self-help and daily living skills. This can range from
problems with dexterity affecting activities such as learning to tie shoelaces to reminders
regarding personal hygiene, dress sense and time management. Clinicians have also
recognised significant problems with adaptive behaviour, especially with regard to
anger management, anxiety and mood. (Attwood 2002). Clinical experience and research
has confirmed that in terms of the child's behavioural profile, children and adults with
High Functioning Autism and Asperger's syndrome have a very similar presentation
(Ozonoff, South, and Miller 2000). Both groups benefit from the same behavioural
treatment programs.

The academic may decide whether a particular subject in a research study has a
diagnosis of autism or Asperger's syndrome to ensure that their research examines the
same clinical populations as in other studies. The clinician has other considerations and
decides whether the child has a diagnosis of autism or Asperger's syndrome to help
define and understand their differences to other children. However their
recommendations for treatment for both High Functioning Autism and Asperger's
syndrome are the same.

Clinicians have noted that as the clinical picture of Pervasive Developmental Disorders
or Autistic Spectrum Disorders changes over time, a child may receive a diagnosis of
severe autism or High Functioning Autism at one point in their developmental history
and Asperger's syndrome at a later stage. (Attwood 1998, Gillberg 1998). There is also
the opinion among clinicians that, contrary to DSM, if a child meets criteria for both
autism and Asperger's syndrome, the child is given a diagnosis of Asperger's syndrome
(Mahoney, Szatmari, MacLean, Bryson, Bartolucci, Walter, Jones and Zwaigenbaum
1998)

A dilemma for the clinician is whether a particular diagnosis enables the child to have
access to the government services that they need. In some countries, a child may only
have support in the classroom or the parents receive government allowances or medical
insurance coverage if the child has a diagnosis of autism. Clinicians may write reports
with a diagnosis of autism rather than the more accurate diagnosis of Asperger's
syndrome. This is particularly relevant when one considers the epidemiological research
suggests that one person in 250 has Asperger's syndrome, using the criteria being
adopted by clinicians (Kadesjo Gillberg and Hagberg 1999). Government and nongovernment
agencies, especially Education and Health departments, have usually not
been funded for such an incidence and are reluctant to 'open the floodgates'.

Conclusion

Having reviewed the literature, we may be able to answer the question, is there a
difference between Asperger's syndrome and High Functioning Autism? The reply is
that the research and clinical experience would suggest that there is no clear evidence
that they are different disorders. Their similarities are greater than their differences.
We
appear to be taking, particularly in Europe and Australia, a dimensional view of autism
and Asperger' syndrome rather than a categorical approach. (Leekam, Libby, Wing
Gould and Gillberg 2000).
At present both terms can be used interchangeably in clinical
practice.
 

References

Attwood T. Asperger's syndrome: A guide for parents and professionals. London Jessica
Kingsley Publications. 1998.
Attwood T. (2002) Frameworks for behavioural interventions. Child and Adolescent
Psychiatric Clinics of North America. 12 (in press).
Dickerson Mayes, S. and Calhoun, S.L. (2001) Non-significance of early speech delay in
children with autism and normal intelligence and implications for DSM-IV Asperger's
Disorder. Autism. 5, 81-94.
Ehler, S., Nyden A., Gillberg C. Dahlgren Sandberg A., Dahlgren S.O., Hjelmquist, E.,
and Oden A. (1997) Asperger syndrome, autism and attention disorders: A comparative
study of the cognitive profiles of 120 children. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 38 207-217.
Eisenmajer, R., Prior, M., Leekam, S., Wing L., Ong, B., Gould, J., and Welham M. (1998)
Delayed language onset as a predictor of clinical symptoms in Pervasive Developmental
Disorders. Journal of Autism and developmental Disorders, 28, 527-533.
Gillberg C. Asperger syndrome and High Functioning Autism. British Journal of
Psychiatry, 171, 200-209.
Howlin P. and Asgharian A. (1999) The diagnosis of autism and Asperger syndrome:
findings from a survey of 770 families. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology,
41, 834-839.
Klin A., Volkmar F.R., , Sparrow S.S., , Cicchetti D. V., and Rourke B.P. (1995) Validity
and neuropsychological characterization of Asperger Syndrome: Convergence with
Nonverbal Learning Disabilities Syndrome. Journal of Child psychology and Psychiatry
36, 1127-40.
Kadesjo B., Gillberg C., and Hagberg B. (1999) Autism and Asperger syndrome in sevenyear-
old children: a total population study. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders, 29, 327-331.
Leekam S., Libby S., Wing L., Gould J., and Gillberg C. (2000) Comparison of ICD-10 and
Gillberg's criteria for Asperger syndrome. Autism 4, 11-28.
Mahoney W. J., Szatmari P., , MacLean J.E., Bryson S. E., Bartolucci G., Walter S.D., Jones
M.B. and Zwaigenbaum L. (1998) Reliability and accuracy of differentiating Pervasive
Developmental Disorder subtypes. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
adolescent Psychiatry, 37, 278-285.
Manjiviona, J. and Prior M. (1999) Neuropsychological profiles of children with
Asperger syndrome and autism. Autism 3, 327-356.
Miller, J.N. and Ozonoff, S. (1997) Did Asperger's cases have Asperger Disorder? A
research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 38. 247-251.
Miller, J.N. and Ozonoff,S. (2000) The external validity of Asperger Disorder: Lack of
evidence from the domain of Neuropsychology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 109,
227-238.
Ozonoff S., South M., and Miller J.N. (2000) DSM-IV defined Asperger syndrome:
Cognitive, behavioural and early history differentiation from High Functioning Autism.
Autism 4, 29-46.

Copywrite 2003 Tony Attwood
 
 

No comments:

Google